Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

No Evidence for the Association between a Polymorphism in the PCLO Depression Candidate Gene with Memory Bias in Remitted Depressed Patients and Healthy Individuals

  • Janna N. Vrijsen ,

    Janna.Vrijsen@radboudumc.nl

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, the Netherlands

  • Anne Speckens,

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, the Netherlands

  • Alejandro Arias-Vásquez,

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, the Netherlands, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, the Netherlands, Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, The Netherlands, Department of Human Genetics, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, The Netherlands

  • Barbara Franke,

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, the Netherlands, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, the Netherlands, Department of Human Genetics, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, The Netherlands

  • Eni S. Becker,

    Affiliation Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands

  • Iris van Oostrom

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, the Netherlands

Abstract

The PCLO rs2522833 candidate polymorphism for depression has been associated to monoaminergic neurotransmission. In healthy and currently depressed individuals, the polymorphism has been found to affect activation of brain areas during memory processing, but no direct association of PCLO with memory bias was found. We hypothesized that the absence of this association might have been obscured by current depressive symptoms or genetically driven individual differences in reactivity to stressful events. Experiencing stressful childhood events fosters dysfunctional assumptions that are related to cognitive biases, and may modulate the predisposition for depression via epigenetic effects. The association between PCLO and memory bias, as well as interaction between PCLO and childhood events was studied in patients remitted from depression (N = 299), as well as a sample of healthy individuals (N = 157). The participants performed an emotional verbal memory task after a sad mood induction. Childhood trauma and adversity were measured with a questionnaire. The Genotype main effect, and Genotype by Childhood Events interaction were analyzed for memory bias in both samples. PCLO risk allele carrying remitted depressed patients did not show more negatively biased memory than non-risk allele carriers, not even patients with stressful childhood events. A similar pattern of results was found in healthy individuals. Memory bias may not be strongly associated with the PCLO rs2522833 polymorphism. We did not find any support for the PCLO-childhood events interaction, but the power of our study was insufficient to exclude this possibility.

Introduction

Depression results in a high disease burden as well as increased mortality risk due to important impairments in social, emotional and physical functioning. Twin studies found that the genetic contribution to depression is up to 40% [1]. The SNP rs2522833 in the piccolo (PCLO) gene has been associated with current depression [2][5]. The PCLO gene encodes a presynaptic cytomatrix protein called piccolo. Specifically, the rs2522833 polymorphism leads to a serine to alanine substitution in the C2A calcium sensor domain of the protein and may affect protein stability [6], [7]. Monoaminergic modulation of the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal system (involved in stress responses) may be a route through which PCLO affects depression vulnerability [4], [8], [9]. The C ‘risk’ allele is more frequent in depressed individuals than in healthy individuals and has been associated with heritable personality traits that predispose to depression in healthy individuals, such as ‘harm avoidance’ [4], as well as with changes in HPA responsiveness after treatment [9]. Other studies, however, were not able to replicate the association with clinical depression [7], or did not find an association with sub-clinical levels of depression [3].

Depression is a complex multifactorial condition and genetic studies so far have yielded mixed results [1]. Cognitive information processing styles associated with depression could be more closely related to genetic factors than the complex and heterogeneous phenotype of major depressive disorder and hence provide another way to study the association between candidate genes and depression [10], [11]. Biased processing of emotional information is considered to be a strong contributor to the development as well as the recurrence of depression, and has also been associated with other forms of psychopathology such as anxiety, eating disorders, and addiction [12][17]. Negatively biased memory, however, seems particularly robust in depression [18][22]. Depressed individuals are inclined to have better memory for negative than positive information, compared to healthy individuals who tend to preferentially recall positive information [18], [19], [23]. Upon remission, sad mood state activates biased processing [24][27]. Biased memory in depression is strongest if the information is self-relevant [16], and is generally studied using explicit memory tasks [28][30].

Emotional processing biases have been associated with several depression candidate genes, such as the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism in the SLC6A4 gene, and the BDNF, COMT and NR3C2 genes (e.g. [31][36]). In addition to being related to depression, the risk (‘C’) allele in the PCLO rs2522833 polymorphism has also been associated with neural correlates of biased processing of emotional faces and memory bias [37][39]. Schott and colleagues [27] reported that C allele carriers had lower (non-emotional) memory performance, which was associated with reduced hippocampal activation. Woudstra and colleagues [39] found the PCLO polymorphism to affect activation of relevant brain regions (i.e. the anterior cingulate circuit, inferior frontal gyrus, and insula) during emotional memory processing in 89 currently depressed and 29 healthy individuals. However, no direct association between the PCLO genotype and emotional memory processing was found.

The complex interplay between genetic susceptibility and stressful life events is considered to be an important mechanism in the development of depression [40][42]. Especially stressful childhood events may be of importance [43]. Experiencing negative events in childhood can lead to dysfunctional basic assumptions about the self and the world [44]. These assumptions may induce cognitive biases, as information is processed in accordance with the (dysfunctional) assumptions. Subsequent stressful events may trigger biased processing, such as negatively biased attention and memory, and may contribute to vulnerability for developing symptoms of depression or to relapse (see cognitive models of depression, [44], [45]). This process may be strongest in genetically susceptible individuals. Indeed, studies have found that depression risk allele carriers who experienced stressful life events generally show stronger negative memory bias and/or less positive memory bias [33], [34], [46]. Hence, stressful events may modulate the genetic risk effect on depressotypic information processing.

The fact that the interaction between genetic susceptibility and stressful events has not been examined so far may have contributed to the null-findings regarding the association of PCLO and memory bias. Furthermore, most of the participants in the Woudstra and colleagues [39] study were depressed at the time of testing. Participants'depressed mood [16], as well as concentration and memory problems associated with depression probably had a strong effect on emotional memory processing in the Woudstra and colleagues [39] study. This may have overshadowed the small effect of PCLO on memory bias. The aim of the current study was to examine the effect of PCLO on memory bias as well as the gene-childhood stress interaction in a group of patients remitted from depression and a group of healthy participants. We included a sample of remitted patients because this offers the possibility to study cognitive processes in individuals highly at risk for a (recurrent) depressive episode [47], [48], while minimizing depressive symptoms or concomitants affecting cognitive biases [49]. The analyses were repeated in a sample of healthy individuals to confirm the pattern of results. We hypothesized to find an association between PCLO rs2522833 variation and memory bias in remitted depressed patients as well as healthy individuals. We expected this effect to be more pronounced in individuals who experienced stressful childhood events than in individuals who did not.

Methods and Materials

Participants

This study is part of a larger study, which aims to link cognitive biases to genetic susceptibility to depression in two independent samples of 360 undergraduate female students of the Radboud University Nijmegen (healthy individuals) and 327 individuals who were remitted from depression (remitted patients). Complete genotype data were available for a total of 351 healthy individuals. Half of the healthy individuals underwent the same sad mood induction procedure as the remitted depressed patients and were therefore selected for the current study. Two of the healthy individuals did not complete the memory task, resulting in a sample of N = 157 for the further analyses. The remitted depressed patients underwent the same sad mood induction. Complete genotype data were available for a total of 320 remitted patients, and complete genotype and memory bias data for a total of 299 remitted patients for further analyses. The remitted patients were recruited at the Department of Psychiatry of the Radboud University Medical Centre and other outpatient psychiatric services in the region. The participants were invited for the study by means of an information letter. Before the onset of the experiment, participants were given the opportunity to ask questions and they were instructed that they could stop at any time, without specification of a reason. Subsequently, they gave written informed consent. Participants were included in the remitted depressed patient group if they met the criteria of the DSM-IV [50] for a previous depressive episode. Exclusion criteria for both groups were: current depressive episode, current or lifetime bipolar disorder, current psychotic symptoms, alcohol or substance abuse within the past 6 months, deafness, blindness, neurological disorder, sensorimotor handicaps and intellectual disability. In order to assess in- and exclusion criteria, trained professionals interviewed eligible participants with the Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-IV Axis-I disorders (SCID-I) [51]. The SCID-I has been demonstrated to have a good reliability [52], [53]. State depressive symptoms were measured with the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II) [54]. Participants received course credit or a gift certificate in return for their participation. Participants were debriefed before they were rewarded for their participation. The study was approved by the Dutch Central Committee on Research involving Human Subjects (CCMO) and we complied with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki) in the treatment of our participants.

Stressful childhood events

Stressful childhood events were assessed with an adapted version of the Life Events Questionnaire [55]. This version has previously been used in comparable studies [33], [34], [56]. Participants were asked to indicate whether they had experienced a set of life events before the age 16 of years, after the age of 16, and within the last year. A childhood traumatic events variable was calculated indicating whether or not participants experienced traumatic events (i.e. physical or sexual abuse) involving a family member or a non-family member before the age of 16 years. Because the prevalence of aggression and/or abuse was rather low in the sample of healthy individuals (16% of all participants) and we expect a similar effect of non-traumatic but highly stressful events, the effect of traumatic as well as stressful adverse childhood events was studied in this sample. This variable indicated whether or not participants experienced traumatic events (aggression and/or abuse) involving a family member or a non-family member and/or adverse events (parental loss, divorce of parents and/or prolonged separation of parents) before the age of 16 [33].

Sad mood induction

Prior to the memory task, participants were presented with a highly emotional sad film segment from the movie “Sophie’s choice” [57]. They were instructed to let the emotionality of the film influence their mood as much as possible and to maintain the sad mood state. Participants rated their mood state using a computerized visual analog scale that ranged from −10 (indicating saddest mood) to 10 (indicating happiest mood) after the mood induction. Remitted patients also rated their mood state before the mood induction.

Memory bias

This memory task has been previously used and described [35], [58]. Participants were presented with 12 depression-specific negative and 12 positive words in fixed order, with no more than two words of the same valence being presented consecutively. Words were selected from two databases (Dutch translation of the Affective Norms for English Words database; [59, and 60]). Each word was presented for 10 seconds in capital black letters against a white background. To make encoding self-referential, participants were instructed to vividly imagine themselves in a scene with the presented word [61]. After completion of this rating, the next word would appear on the screen. This task was followed by a short paper-and-pencil distraction task (Raven matrices) [62]. After that, participants were instructed to return to the computer for an unannounced free recall of the words from the previous computer task. Participants were instructed to type in all the words they could remember within three minutes. A difference-score representing memory bias was calculated by subtracting the number of correctly recalled negative words from the number of correctly recalled positive words. A negative score represented negative memory bias.

Genotyping

In healthy individuals, saliva samples were collecting using Oragene kit (DNA Genotek, Inc. Ottawa, Ontario, Canada). In remitted patients, blood was taken by venipuncture and DNA was isolated using standard protocols. Molecular analyses were performed in a CCKL-accredited laboratory at the Department of Human Genetics of the Radboud University Medical Centre. The PCLO rs2522833 polymorphism was genotyped using TaqMan analysis [63]. Genotyping failed for two remitted patients and nine healthy individuals, resulting in a call rate of 99.4% for remitted patients and 97.5% for healthy individuals, respectively. The PCLO allele frequency distribution in healthy individuals and remitted patients is presented in Table 1. Testing for Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium did not show deviation from the expected distribution of genotypes in healthy individuals, N = 351, χ2 = .10, df = 1, p = .75, nor in remitted patients, N = 320, χ2 = .03, df = 1, p = .85. Genotype groups were created in line with Woudstra and colleagues [39] and the association with depression: ‘C’ risk allele carriers (homozygous and heterozygous for C allele) versus ‘A’ non-risk allele carriers (homozygous for A allele).

thumbnail
Table 1. The PCLO allele frequency distribution presented by number of participants and percentage of participants in remitted patients and healthy individuals.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0112153.t001

Statistical analyses

The effects of PCLO genotype (‘C’ risk allele carriers, ‘A’ non-risk allele homozygotes), childhood trauma/adversity (no, yes) and their interaction on memory bias in remitted patients and healthy individuals were examined using analysis of co-variance (ANCOVA). In line with the Woudstra and colleagues [39] study, we also examined the two samples together. Because of their known effect on cognitive functioning and emotional processing, age and sex were included as covariates in the analyses.

Results

Sample descriptive and mood induction

Descriptives of the two independent samples are presented in Table 2. Pre mood induction mood ratings of 14 remitted patients were missing due to a programming error. The mood induction was successful in remitted patients as mood state decreased significantly from pre (M = 3.47 S.D.3.62) to post (M = .27 S.D. = 4.52), F(1,284) = 236.91, p<.001, Cohen's f = .91. The effect in healthy participants could not be evaluated because we did not have a baseline mood measure.

thumbnail
Table 2. Demographic and childhood events variables of the two independent samples. Childhood trauma refers to physical or sexual abuse before the age of 16 years and childhood adversity refers to childhood trauma and parental loss, divorce of parents and/or prolonged separation of parents before the age of 16. SD refers to standard deviation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0112153.t002

PCLO genotype main effect

Neither in remitted patients, F(1,293)  = 1.89, p = .170, f = .08, nor in healthy individuals, F(1,152)  = .83, p = .363, f = .07. PCLO a main effect of PCLO genotype on memory bias was found. The analysis of the whole two samples together did not yield an effect on memory bias either , F(1,450)  = 2.15, p = .144, f = .06.

PCLO genotype x stressful childhood events interaction

The mean memory bias difference scores (# correctly recalled positive words - # correctly recalled negative words) are presented in Table 3. In remitted patients, genotype did not significantly interact with childhood trauma on memory bias, F(1,293)  = 1.18, p = .184, f = .08. The results are similar when examining the interaction between genotype and childhood adversity (as in healthy individuals) instead of childhood trauma in remitted patients, F(1,293)  = .14, p = .713, f = .00. The interaction between genotype and childhood adversity was not significant in healthy individuals either, F(1,152)  = 3.09, p = .081, f = .14.

thumbnail
Table 3. Mean memory bias difference scores (# correctly recalled positive words - # correctly recalled negative words) and standard deviations (SD) for the PCLO genotype (risk allele ‘C’ carriers vs. non-risk allele ‘A’-homozygotes) x childhood trauma interaction in remitted depressed individuals, and for the PCLO genotype x childhood adversity interaction in healthy individuals.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0112153.t003

Restricting the analyses to female patients with remitted depression (N = 193) did not make any difference in terms of PCLO main effect, F(1,188)  = .93, p = .336, f = .07, or interaction with childhood trauma, F(1,188)  = .42, p = .520, f = .04.

No significant effect was found when examining the genotype by childhood adversity interaction in the two samples together, F(1,450)  = .57, p = .451, f = .03. To check whether our non-significant results were due to a lack of statistical power, we conducted post hoc power analyses using Quanto power calculator (http://hydra.usc.edu/gxe/). The power analyses revealed that in order for a PCLO main effect of this size to be detected (80% chance) as significant at the 5% level, a sample of 134 participants would be required, and 2153 participants for an interaction effect.

Discussion

Previous studies have found an association between the PCLO rs2522833 polymorphism and neural correlates of biased processing in healthy and currently depressed individuals, but no direct effect of the PCLO genotype on memory bias [38], [39]. Memory bias has been associated with other depression candidate genes, specifically in individuals who experienced stressful life events (e.g. [33], [34]). Therefore, we hypothesized that the absence of the association between the PCLO genotype and memory bias in previous studies might have been due to genetically driven individual differences in reactivity to environmental stress [41], as well as to current depressive symptoms. The association between the PCLO rs2522833 polymorphism, memory bias and the modulating effect of stressful childhood events was examined in a sample of remitted depressed patients and a healthy sample. We found no evidence for an association between the PCLO rs2522833 polymorphism and memory bias, nor for an interaction effect of the polymorphism with stressful childhood events. Our findings add to the current knowledge and suggest that the absence of an association between PCLO and memory bias may not be explained by current depressive symptoms, or by a gene-childhood stress interaction. However, the current study did not have sufficient power to detect the PCLO-childhood stress interaction and may have resulted in false negative findings. Therefore, we cannot rule out the possibility that the interaction effect exists, but was not detected due to a lack of power. A larger sample is required to confirm these results. Taken together, results from our study and the recent study by Woudstra and colleagues [39] indicate that verbal emotional memory bias may not be strongly associated with the PCLO rs2522833 polymorphism - unlike other genes that have been associated with both memory bias and depression (e.g. BDNF, NR3C2) - and may be implicated in other routes towards the susceptibility for depression.

It has been proposed that the PCLO rs2522833 polymorphism may be one of the genetic variants that increase the risk for depression by affecting the stress regulation capacity of the brain, possibly by altering monoaminergic neurotransmission, especially serotonin [39]. The imaging studies offer support for this [38], [39]. Variation in the PCLO rs2522833 polymorphism was associated with amygdala activation during emotion processing, as well as lower striatum activation during emotional memory processing in depressed individuals. Altered activation in these neuronal circuits is related to depression and emotional information processes [64]. Based on the endophenotype model [10], the path from gene to brain activation is shorter and the link stronger, compared to the path from gene to cognitive measures, such as memory bias. Hence, the effects of PCLO may be too subtle to show up on a cognitive measure, while effects can be found on neural correlates of biased processing. We included, however, a larger sample than the previous study to examine the association of PCLO with memory bias [39] and we had sufficient power to detect a main effect of the PCLO polymorphism. The null-findings may therefore imply that the PCLO rs2522833 polymorphism is not related to emotional information processing that is associated with the susceptibility for depression.

Previous studies found an association between depression and the PCLO rs2522833 polymorphism when studying current major depressive disorder, but not for sub-clinical levels of depression [3], [7], [65]. To illustrate, in the study by Hek and colleagues [3], evidence for the association with depression was found in a well-diagnosed clinical subsample, but not in a sample of individuals with minor depression, self-reported depression, depression diagnosed by a general practitioner, or individuals using antidepressant medication. Sullivan and colleagues [7] also only found an association between PCLO and depression in a homogeneous clinically depressed subsample. Patients included in our study fulfilled the clinical criteria of one or more depressive episodes in the past. Our null-findings can therefore not be attributed to the selection of a too heterogeneous sample with subclinical levels of depression. However, it is important to note that we studied PCLO in relation to information processing styles that are associated with depression and the susceptibility for depression only, and not the association between PCLO and depressive disorder, as in the studies presented above.

Our results should be viewed in the context of some strengths and limitations. Strengths of our study are that we used two samples and that studying gene-bias associations in an affected sample is rather novel. Another strength of the study is that we avoided current symptoms overruling subtle cognitive effects by testing groups that are currently not depressed. A limitation is the difference in sex distribution between the two samples, as SNPs within the PCLO gene have been associated with depression in females and not in males [66]. The fact that only female healthy individuals were included, limits the generalizability of these results. Another limitation may be the measurement method of the childhood events, as negative bias may have affected the participants' recall. The Life Events Questionnaire assesses the occurrence of several factual events during pre-determined periods of life. Because such specific events were assessed and generally adults' recall of specific childhood events is fairly accurate [67], it is in our opinion unlikely that recall had been distorted.

In conclusion, the presence of the PCLO risk allele may increase vulnerability for depression, but we found no indication for an association with memory bias. Memory bias may not be strongly associated with the PCLO rs2522833 polymorphism, unlike other genes that have been associated with depression (e.g. BDNF, NR3C2). Due to the insufficient power to detect a PLCO-childhood trauma interaction effect, we however cannot rule out that PLCO is associated with memory bias in individuals with stressful childhood events.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the participants for their time and effort.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: JV AS EB IVO. Performed the experiments: JV. Analyzed the data: JV AAV IVO. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: JV. Wrote the paper: JV AS AAV BF IVO.

References

  1. 1. Sullivan PF, Neale MC, Kendler KS (2000) Genetic epidemiology of major depression: review and metaanalysis. Am J Psychiat 157: 1552–1562.
  2. 2. Bochdanovits Z, Verhage M, Smit AB, de Geus EJ, Posthuma D, et al. (2009) Joint reanalysis of 29 correlated SNPs supports the role of PCLO/Piccolo as a causal risk factor for major depressive disorder. Mol Psychiatr 14: 650–652.
  3. 3. Hek K, Mulder CL, Luijendijk HJ, van Duijn CM, Hofman A, et al. (2010) The PCLO gene and depressive disorders: replication in a population-based study. Hum Mol Genet 19: 731–734.
  4. 4. Minelli A, Scassellati C, Cloninger CR, Tessari E, Bortolomasi M, et al. (2012) PCLO gene: its role in vulnerability to major depressive disorder. J Affect Disord 139: 250–255.
  5. 5. Verbeek EC, Bevova MR, Bochdanovits Z, Rizzu P, Bakker IM, et al. (2013) Resequencing three candidate genes for major depressive disorder in a dutch cohort. PLoS One 8: e79921.
  6. 6. Betts MJ, Russell RB (2007) Amino-Acid Properties and Consequences of Substitutions, in Bioinformatics for Geneticists: A Bioinformatics Primer for the Analysis of Genetic Data, 2nd ed. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd, Chichester, UK. In: Barnes MR, editors. Bioinformatics for Geneticists: A Bioinformatics Primer for the Analysis of Genetic Data. Chichester, UK, Ltd, John Wiley & Sons 289–316.
  7. 7. Sullivan PF, de Geus EJ, Willemsen G, James MR, Smit JH, et al. (2009) Genome-wide association for major depressive disorder: a possible role for the presynaptic protein piccolo. Mol Psychiatry 14: 359–375.
  8. 8. Kuehner C, Huffziger S, Witt SH, Rietschel M (2011) PCLO rs 2522833 impacts HPA system activity in healthy young adults. Transl. Psychiatry 1: e10.
  9. 9. Schuhmacher A, Mössner R, Höfels S, Pfeiffer U, Guttenthaler V, et al. (2011) PCLO rs2522833 modulates HPA system response to antidepressant treatment in major depressive disorder. Int J Neuropsychoph 14: 237–245.
  10. 10. Franke B, Neale BM, Faraone SV (2009) Genome-wide association studies in ADHD. Human Genetics 126: 13–50.
  11. 11. Kendler KS, Neale MC (2010) Endophenotype: a conceptual analysis. Mol Psychiatr 15: 789–797.
  12. 12. De Raedt R, Koster EH (2010) Understanding vulnerability for depression from a cognitive neuroscience perspective: a reappraisal of attentional factors and a new conceptual framework. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 10: 50–70.
  13. 13. Klein AA, Nelson LM, Anker JJ (2013) Attention and recognition memory bias for alcohol-related stimuli among alcohol-dependent patients attending residential treatment. Addict Behav 38: 1687–1690.
  14. 14. LeMoult J, Joormann J (2012) Attention and memory biases in social anxiety disorder: The role of comorbid depression. Cognitive Ther Res 36: 47–57.
  15. 15. Mathews A, MacLeod C (2005) Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Annu Rev Clin Psycho 1: 167–95.
  16. 16. Gotlib IH, Joormann J (2010) Cognition and depression: Current status and future directions. Annu Rev Clin Psycho 6: 285–312.
  17. 17. Wingenfeld K, Terfehr K, Meyer B, Löwe B, Spitzer C (2013) Memory bias for emotional and illness-related words in patients with depression, anxiety and somatization disorders: an investigation with the directed forgetting task. Psychopathology 46: 22–27.
  18. 18. Matt GE, Vazquez C, Campbell WK (1992) Mood-congruent recall of affectively toned stimuli: A meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev 12: 227–255.
  19. 19. Ridout N, Noreen A, Johal J (2009) Memory for emotional faces in naturally occurring dysphoria and induced sadness. Behav Res Ther 47: 851–860.
  20. 20. Vrijsen JN, Becker ES, Arias-Vásquez A, van Dijk MK, Speckens A, et al. (2014a) What is the contribution of different cognitive biases and stressful childhood events to the presence and number of previous depressive episodes? Psychiat Res 217: 134–42.
  21. 21. Williams JMG (1996) Depression and the specificity of autobiographical memory. Remembering our past: Studies in autobiographical memory. New York, NY, Cambridge University Press.
  22. 22. Williams JMG, Barnhofer T, Crane C, Hermans D, Raes F, et al. (2007) Autobiographical memory specificity and emotional disorder. Psychol Bull 133: 122–148.
  23. 23. Gotlib IH, Kasch KL, Traill S, Joormann J, Arnow BA, et al. (2004) Coherence and specificity of information processing biases in depression and social phobia. J Abnorm Psychol 113: 386–398.
  24. 24. Gupta R, Kar BR (2012) Attention and memory biases as stable abnormalities among currently depressed and currently remitted individuals with unipolar depression. Front Psychiatry 3: 99.
  25. 25. Joormann J, Gotlib IH (2007) Selective attention to emotional faces following recovery from depression. J Abnorm Psychol 116: 80–85.
  26. 26. McCabe SB, Gotlib IH, Martin RA (2000) Cognitive vulnerability for depression: Deployment of attention as a function of history of depression and current mood state. Cognitive Ther Res 24: 427–444.
  27. 27. Teasdale JD (1988) Cognitive vulnerability to persistent depression. Cognition and Emotion 2: 247–274.
  28. 28. Denny EB, Hunt RR (1992) Affective valence and memory in depression: dissociation of recall and fragment completion. J Abnorm Psychol 101: 575–580.
  29. 29. Ellwart T, Rinck M, Becker ES (2003) Selective memory and memory deficits in depressed inpatients. Depress Anxiety 17: 197–206.
  30. 30. Watkins PC, Mathews A, Williamson DA, Fuller RD (1992) Mood-congruent memory in depression: emotional priming or elaboration? J Abnorm Psychol 101: 581–586.
  31. 31. Fox E, Ridgewell A, Ashwin C (2009) Looking on the bright side: Biased attention and the human serotonin transporter gene. Proceedings in Biological Science 276: 1747–1751.
  32. 32. Pergamin-Hight L, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH, Bar-Haim Y (2012) Variations in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter gene and biased attention for emotional information: a meta-analysis. Biol Psychiat 71: 373–379.
  33. 33. van Oostrom I, Franke B, Rijpkema M, Gerritsen L, Arias-Vasquez A, et al. (2012) Interaction between BDNF Val66Met and childhood stressful life events is associated to affective memory bias in men but not women. Biol Psychol 89: 214–219.
  34. 34. Vogel S, Gerritsen L, van Oostrom I, Arias-Vásquez A, Rijpkema M, et al. (2014) Linking genetic variants of the mineralocorticoid receptor and negative memory bias: interaction with prior life adversity. Psychoneuroendocrino 40: 181–190..
  35. 35. Vrijsen JN, van Oostrom I, Arias-Vásquez A, Franke B, Becker ES, et al. (2014 b) Association between genes, stressful childhood events and processing bias in depression vulnerable individuals. Genes Brain Behav 13: 508–516.
  36. 36. Williams LM, Gatt JM, Grieve SM, Dobson-Stone C, Paul RH, et al. (2010) COMT Val(108/158)Met polymorphism effects on emotional brain function and negativity bias. Neuroimage 53: 918–925.
  37. 37. Schott BH, Assmann A, Schmierer P, Soch J, Erk A, et al. (2014) Epistatic interaction of genetic depression risk variants in the human subgenual cingulate cortex during memory encoding. Tansl Psychiatry 4: e372.
  38. 38. Woudstra S, Bochdanovits Z, van Tol MJ, Veltman DJ, Zitman FG, et al. (2012) Piccolo genotype modulates neural correlates of emotion processing but not executive functioning. Translational Psychiatry 2: e99.
  39. 39. Woudstra S, van Tol MJ, Bochdanovits Z, van der Wee NJ, Zitman FG, et al. (2013) Modulatory Effects of the Piccolo Genotype on Emotional Memory in Health and Depression. PLoS One 8: e61494.
  40. 40. Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Craig IW, et al. (2003) Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science 301: 386–389.
  41. 41. Caspi A, Hariri AR, Holmes A, Uher R, Moffitt TE (2010) Genetic sensitivity to the environment: the case of the serotonin transporter gene and its implications for studying complex diseases and traits. Am J Psychiat 167: 509–527.
  42. 42. Caspi A, Moffitt TE (2006) Gene-environment interactions in psychiatry: joining forces with neuroscience. Nature Review in Neuroscience 7: 583–590.
  43. 43. Perry B, Pollard R, Blakley T, Baker W, Vigilante D (1995) Childhood trauma, the neurobiology of adaptation, and “use-dependent” development of the brain: how “states” become “traits.”. Infant Ment Health J 16: 271–91.
  44. 44. Beck AT (2008) The evolution of the cognitive model of depression and its neurobiological correlates. Am J Psychiatry 165: 969–977.
  45. 45. Bower GH (1981) Mood and memory. American Psychologist 36: 129–148.
  46. 46. Firk C, Markus CR (2007) Review: Serotonin by stress interaction: A susceptibility factor for the development of depression? J Psychopharmacol 21: 538–544.
  47. 47. Bockting C, Spinhoven P, Koeter M, Wouters L, Schene A, for the Depression Evaluation Longitudinal Therapy Assessment (DELTA) Study Group (2006) Prediction of recurrence in recurrent depression and the influence of consecutive episodes on vulnerability for depression: a 2-year prospective study. J Clin Psychiat 67: 747–755.
  48. 48. Kanai T, Takeuchi H, Furukawa TA, Yoshimura R, Imaizumi T, et al. (2003) Time to recurrence after recovery from major depressive episodes and its predictors. Psychol Med 33: 839–845.
  49. 49. Bhagwagar Z, Cowen PJ (2007) It's not over when it's over: Persistent neurobiological abnomalities in recovered depressed patients. Psychol Med 38: 307–313.
  50. 50. American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC.
  51. 51. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW (1996) Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Axis I Disorders-Clinician Version (SCID-CV) American Psychiatric Press, Washington, DC.
  52. 52. Skre I, Onstad S, Torgersen S, Kringlen E (1991) High interrater reliability for the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–III–R Axis I (SCID-I) Acta Psychiat Scand. 84: 167–173.
  53. 53. Williams JB, Gibbon M, First MB, Spitzer RL, Davis M, et al. (1992) The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–III–R (SCID): Multisite test–retest reliability. Arch Gen Psychiat 49: 630–636.
  54. 54. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK (1996) Beck Depression Inventory manual. 2nd ed. Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX.
  55. 55. Brugha TS, Cragg D (1990) The list of threatening experiences-the reliability and validity of a brief life events questionnaire. Acta Psychiat Scand 82: 77–81.
  56. 56. Gerritsen L, Tendolkar I, Franke B, Vasquez AA, Kooijman S, et al. (2011) BDNF Val66Met genotype modulates the effect of childhood adversity on subgenual anterior cingulate cortex volume in healthy subjects. Mol Psychiat 17: 597–603.
  57. 57. Fitzgerald DA, Arnold JF, Becker ES, Speckens AE, Rinck M, et al. (2011) How mood challenges emotional memory formation: an fMRI investigation. Neuroimage 56: 1783–1790.
  58. 58. Vrijsen JN, van Oostrom I, Isaac L, Becker ES, Speckens A, (2014 c)Coherence between attentional and memory biases in sad and formerly depressed individuals. Cognitive Therapy and Research 38: 334–342.
  59. 59. Bradley MM, Lang PJ (1999) Affective norms for English words (ANEW): Instruction manual and affective ratings. Technical Report C-1, The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.
  60. 60. Phaf RH, van der Leij AR, Stienen BMC, Bierman D (2006) Positieve, neutrale en negatieve woorden bij minimale aanbieding: Een ordening door perceptuele clarificatie [Positive, neutral and negative words at minimal presentation levels: Ordering by perceptual clarification]. Technical Report, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, Universiteit van Amsterdam.
  61. 61. Rogers T, Kuiper N, Kirker W (1977) Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 35: 677–688.
  62. 62. Raven JC (1958) Standard progressive matrices. H. K. Lewis, London.
  63. 63. Watson DE, Li B (2005) TaqMan applications in genetic and molecular toxicology. Int J Toxicol 24: 139–145.
  64. 64. van Tol MJ, Demenescu LR, van der Wee NJA, Kortekaas R, Marjan MA, et al. (2012) Functional magnetic resonance imaging correlates of emotional word encoding and recognition in depression and anxiety disorders. Biol Psychiatry 71: 593–602.
  65. 65. Shyn SI, Hamilton SP (2010) The genetics of major depression: moving beyond the monoamine hypothesis. Psychiatr Clin North Am 33: 125–140.
  66. 66. Aragam N, Wang KS, Pan Y (2011) Genome-wide association analysis of gender differences in major depressive disorder in the Netherlands NESDA and NTR population-based samples. J Affect Disord 133: 516–521.
  67. 67. Brewin CR, Andrews B, Gotlib IH (1993) Psychopathology and early experiences: A reappraisal of retrospective reports. Psychological Bulletin 113: 82–98.